Margaret Cychosz, Ph.D.

Margaret Cychosz, Ph.D.

Meet the Researcher

Cychosz received her doctorate in linguistics from the University of California, Berkeley, and completed postdoctoral training at the Center for Comparative and Evolutionary Biology of Hearing/University of Maryland Cochlear Implant Center for Excellence. She is currently an assistant professor of linguistics at UCLA where her lab specializes in spoken language development in infants and children, especially those who receive cochlear implants. A 2025 Emerging Research Grants scientist, Cychosz is the recipient of an Elizabeth M. Keithley, Ph.D. Early Stage Investigator Award.

Infants learn spoken language by processing thousands of words each day, a challenge even greater for those with cochlear implants due to the limited cues in electrical hearing. As a result, children with cochlear implants often lag behind their peers in speech, language, and literacy.

My lab focuses on identifying early predictors of language development in these children, particularly how caregiver language at home may impact children’s speech production and vocabulary. Our research may lead to simple, caregiver-focused therapies and tools to detect language delays in infants with cochlear implants earlier.

This work builds off of years I’ve spent modeling children’s spoken language development using infant- and child- centered audio recorders. Basically, we have the child wear a tiny audio recorder in a special shirt so we can record everything the child hears and says for days at a time. Then we develop natural language processing algorithms to mine that data and model children’s language development.

For my doctoral research, I used methods like this to model bilingual language development among children acquiring Quechua and Spanish in Bolivia. I saw how they exhibited such large variations in their language development milestones. Now I’ve applied this to understand development among children with hearing loss, in order to intervene before they are severely language delayed.

My great-grandmother (whom I’m named after!) was an accomplished pianist before being struck by typhoid fever in her early 20s. The disease took 80 percent of her hearing. Her future in-laws wanted my great-grandfather to call off the engagement because they didn’t believe that a person who is deaf could be a wife or mother. She never received audiological care or wore hearing aids, even when older. Instead, she learned to lipread—and kept playing piano!

In college I worked as a nanny for a Catalan family living outside of Barcelona. Most children there learn Catalan at home and eventually acquire Spanish at school. It was my first experience watching language development unfold.

I’ve always been an advocate for children’s education, and I’m from a family of public school teachers. I am glad I also can sometimes help families of children with hearing loss advocate for inclusive listening and learning environments in the classroom by providing data as evidence.

I’m a fervent reader. It’s a source of relaxation but it also helps spark unexpected connections. For instance, reading about adaptive evolution within animal species leads to insights about the social evolution and emergence of the speech capacity between caregivers and infants, which is a topic of high relevance for my research program.

Margaret Cychosz, Ph.D., is the recipient of an Elizabeth M. Keithley, Ph.D. Early Stage Investigator Award. HHF is grateful to its community of donors for supporting the full range of hearing and balance science.

Click here to download this Meet the Researcher profile.


The Research

University of California, Los Angeles

Leveraging automatic speech recognition algorithms to understand how the home listening environment impacts spoken language development among infants with cochlear implants

To develop spoken language, infants must rapidly process thousands of words spoken by caregivers around them each day. This is a daunting task, even for typical hearing infants. It is even harder for infants with cochlear implants as electrical hearing compromises many critical cues for speech perception and language development. The challenges that infants with cochlear implants face have long-term consequences: Starting in early childhood, cochlear implant users perform 1-2 standard deviations below peers with typical hearing on nearly every measure of speech, language, and literacy. My lab investigates how children with hearing loss develop spoken language despite the degraded speech signal that they hear and learn language from.

This project addresses the urgent need to identify predictors of speech-language development for pediatric cochlear implant users in infancy. In accordance with social interaction theories of child development, infants with typical hearing who receive more language input have better speech-language outcomes. We amend this theory for the unique listening experience of cochlear implant users and instead hypothesize that infants with cochlear implants who receive more audible language input in the home will be more advanced on two critical precursors to spoken language: speech production and vocabulary development. Study results may translate into inexpensive, caregiver-centered therapies that could be administered during aural rehabilitation to improve children’s spoken language. Furthermore, the large-scale annotated corpus of child speech may serve as training data for a novel speech classification algorithm that can be used to diagnose speech-language delay for infants with cochlear implants, years before many current testing regimes permit.

Long-term goal: Data from this project will allow us to (1) understand how different learning environments (the home, daycare) impact spoken language development in young children with hearing loss and (2) develop a novel child speech classification algorithm that will identify children with cochlear implants who are at risk of language delay early in infancy. Speech-language delays among children with cochlear implants are common, but are often not diagnosed until the preschool years, or later, when the child can respond to standardized behavioral testing. If children could be identified earlier, ideally using inexpensive methods that are behaviorally-tailored to infants, we could intervene earlier and provide a more systematic precision medicine approach to each child.